Chapter One: The origins of language
The suspicion does not
appear improbable that the progenitors of man, either the males or females, or
both sexes, before they had acquired the power of expressing their mutual love in
articulate language, endeavoured to charm each other with musical notes and
rhythm. ( Darwin 1871 )
In Charles
Darwin’s vision of the origins of language, early humans had already developed
musical ability prior to language and were using it “to charm each other.” This
may not match the typical image that most of us have our early ancestors as
rather rough characters wearing animal skins and not very charming, but it is
an interesting speculation about how language may have originated. It remains,
however, a speculations.
We
simply don’t know how language originated. We do know that the ability to
produce sound and simple vocal patterning (a hum versus a grunt, for example)
appears to be in an ancient part of the brain that we share with all
vertebrates, including fish, frog, birds and other mammals. But that isn’t
human language. We suspect that some type of spoken language must have
developed between 100,000 and 50,000 years ago, well before written language
(about 5,000 years ago). Yet, among the traces of earlier periods of life on
earth, we never find any direct evidence or artifacts relating to the speech of
our distant ancestors that might tell us how language was back in early stages.
Perhaps because of this absence of direct physical evidence, there has been no
shortage of speculation about the origins of human speech.
The divine source
In
the biblical tradition, as described in the book of Genesis, God created Adam
and “whatsoever Adam called every living creature, that was the name thereof.”
Alternatively, following a Hindu tradition, language came from Sarasvati, wife
of Brahma, creator of the universe. In most religions, there appears to be a
divine source who provides humans with language. In an attempt to rediscover
this original divine langage, a few experiments have ben carried out, with
rather conflicting results. The basic hypothesis seems to have been that, if
human infants were allowed to grow up without hearing any language around them,
then they would spontaneously begin using the orginal God-given language.
The
Greek writer Herodotus reported the story of an Egyptian pharoh named Psammerichus
(or Psamtik) who tried the experiment with two newborn babies more than 2,500
yeasrs ago. After two years of isolation except for the company of goats and a
mute shepherd, the children were reported to have spontaneously uttered, not an
Egyptian word, but something that was identified as the Phygian word bekos, meaning “bread.” The Pharaoh
concluded that Phrygian, an older language spoken in part of what is modern
Turkey, must be the original language. That seems very unlikely. The children
may not have picked up this “word” from any human source, but as several
commentators have pointed out, they must have heard what the goats wear saying.
(first remove the –kos ending, which was added in the Greek version of the
story, then pronounce be as you would the English word bed without –d at the end. Can you hear a goat?
King
James the Fourth of Scotland carried out a similar experiment around the year
1500 and the children were reported to have spontaneously started speaking
Hebrew, confirming the King’s belief that Hebrew had indeed been language of
the Garden of Eden. It is unfortunate that all other cases of children who have
been discovered living in isolation, without coming into contact with human
speech, tend not to confirm the results of these types of divine-source
experiments. Very young children living without access to human language in
their early years grow up with no language at all. If human language did
emanate from a divine source, we have no way of reconstructing that original
language, especially given the events in a place babel “because the lord did
there confound the language of all the earth,” as described in the book of
Genesis in the Bible (11:9)
The natural sound source
A
quite different view of the beginning of language is based on the concept of
natural sounds. The basic idea is that primitive words could have been
imitations of the natural sounds which early men and women heard around them.
When an object flew by, making a caw-caw sound, the early human tried to
imitate the sound and used it to refer to the thing associated with the sound.
And even another flying creature made a coo-coo sound, that natural sounds was
adopted to refer to that kind of object. The fact that all modern languages
have some words with pronunciations that seem to echo naturally occurring
sounds could be used to support this theory. In English, in addition to cuckoo,
we have splash, bang, boom, rattle, buzz, hiss, screech, and forms such as bow-bow.
In fact, this type of few has been called the “bow-bow theory” of language
origin. Words that sound similar to the noises they describe are examples of
anoma-topeia. While it is true that number of words in any language are
onomatopoeic, it is hard to see how most of the soundless things as well as
abstract concept in our world could have been referred to in a language that
simply echoed natural sounds. We might also be rather skeptical about a view
that seems to assume that a language is only a set of words used as “names” for
things.
It
has also been suggested that the original sounds of language may have come from
natural cries of emotion such as pain, anger, and joy. By this route,
presumably, Ouch! came to have it’s painful connotations. But Ouch! and other
interjections such as Ah! Ooh! Wou! or Yuck!, are usually produced with sudden
intakes of breth, wich is the opposite of ordinary talk. We normally produce
spoken language on exhaled breath. Basically, the expressive noises people make
in emotional reactions contain sounds that are not otherwise used in speech
production and consequently would seem to be rather unlikely candidates as
source sounds for language.
The social interaction source
Another
proposal involving natural sounds has been called the “yo-he-yo” theory. The
idea is that the sounds of a person involved in physical effort could be the
source of our language, especially when that physical effort involved several
people and the interaction had to be coordinatd. So, a group of early humans
might develop a set of hums, grunts, groans and curses that were used when they
were lifting and carrying large bits of trees of lifeless hairy mammoths.
The
appeal of this proposal is that is places the development of human language in
a social context. Early people must have lived in groups, if only because
larger groups offered better protection from attack. Groups are necessarily
social organizations and, to maintain those organization, some form of
communication is required, even if it is just grunts and curses,. So, human
sounds, however they they were produced, must have had some principled use
within the life and social interaction of early human goups. This is an
important idea that may relate to the uses of humanly produced sounds. It does
not, however, answer our question regarding the origins of the sounds produced.
Apes and other primates live in social and use grunts and social calls, but
they do not seem to have developed the capacity for speech.
The physical adaptation source
Instead
of looking at types of sounds as the source of human speech, we can look at the
types of physical features human possess, especially those that are distinct
from other creatures, which may have been able to support speech production. We
can start with the observation that, at some early stage, our ancestors made a
very significant transition to an upright posture, with bipedal (on two feet)
locomotion, and a revised role for the front limbs.
Some
effects of this type of change can be seen in physical differences between the
skull of a gorilla and that of a Neandertal man from around 60,000 years ago.
The reconstructed vocal tract of a Neandertal suggest that some consonant-like
sound distinctions would have been possible. We have to wait until about 35,000
years aago for features in reconstructions of fossilized skeletal structures
that begin to resemble those of modern humans. In the study of evolutionary
development, these are certain physical features, best thought of as partial
adaptations, which paper to be relevant for speech. They are streamlined
version of features found in other primates. By themselves, such features would
not necessarily lead to speech production, but they are good clues that a
creature possessing such features probably has the capability for speech.
Teeth,
lips, mouth, larynx and pharynx
Human
teeth are upright, not slanting outwards like those of apes, and they are
roughly even in height. Such characteristics are not useful for ripping or
tearng food and seem better adapted for grinding and chewing. They are also
very helpful in making sounds such as f or v. Human lips have much more
intricate muscle interlacing than is found in other primates and their
resulting flexibility certainly helps in making sounds like p or b. The human
mouth is relatively small compared to other primates, can be open and closed
rapidly, and contains a smaller, thicker and more muscular tongue which can be
used to shape a wide variety of sounds inside the oral cavity. In addition,
unlike other primates, human can close off the airway through the nose to
create more air pressure in the mouth. The overall effect of these small
differences taken together is a face with more intricate muscle interlasing in
the lips and mouth, capable of a wider range of shapes and a more rapid and
powerfull delivery of sounds produced through these different shapes.
The
human larynx or “voice box” (containing the vocal folds or vocal cords) differs
significantly in position from the larynx of other primates such as monkeys. In
the course of human physical development, the assumption of an upright posture
moved the head more directly above the spinal column and the larynx dropped to
a lower position. This created a longer cavity called the pharynx, above the
vocal folds, which acts as a resonator for incrased range and clarity of the sounds
produced via the larynx and the vocal tract. One unfortunate consequence of
this development is that the lower position of the human larynx makes it much
more possible for the human to choke on pieces of food. Monkeys may not be able
to use their larynx to produce speech sounds, but they do not suffer from the
problem of getting food stuck in their windpipe. In evolutionary terms, there
must have been a big advantage in
getting this extra vocal power (i. e. a larger range of sound distinctions) to outweight
the potential disadvantage from an increades risk of choking to death.
The tool-making source
In
the physical adaptation view, one function (producing speech sounds) must have
been superimposed on existing anatomical features (teeth, lips) previously used
for other purpose (chewing, sucking). A similar development is believed to have
taken place with human hands and some believe that manual gestures may have
been a precursor of a language. By about two million years ago, there is
evidence that humans had developed preferential right-handedness and had become
capable of making stone tools. Woods tools and composite tools eventually
followed. Tool-making, or the outcome of manipulating objects and changing them
using both hands, in evidence of a brain at work.
The
human brain is not only large relative to human body size, it is also
lateralized, that is, it has specialized functions in each of the two
hemispheres. Those functions that control the motor movements involved in
complex vocalizations (speaking) and
object manipulation (making or using tools) are very close to each other in the
left hemisphere of the brain. It may be that there was an evolutionary connections
between the language-using and tool-using abilities of humans and that both
were involved in the development of the speaking brain. Most of the other
speculative proposals concerning the origins of speech seem to be based on a
picture of humans producing single noises to indicate objects n their
environment. This activity may indeed have been a crucil stage in the
development of language, but what it lacks is any structural organization.
Alllanguages, including sign language, require the organizing and combining of
sounds of signs in specific arrangements. We seem to have developed a part of our brain that specializes in
making these arrangements.
If
we think in terms of the most basic process involved in primitive tool making,
it is not enough to be able to grasp one rock (make one sound); the human must
also be able to bring another rock (other sounds) into proper contact with the
first in order to develop a tool. In
terms of language structure, the human may have first developed naming ability
by producing a a specific and consisted noise (e.g.bEEr) for a specific object.
The crucial additional step was to bring another specific noise (e.g.gOOd) into
combination with the first build a complex message (bEEr gOOd). Several
thousand years of development later, humans have hones this message-bulding
capacity to a point where, on Saturday watching a football game, they can drink
a sustaining beverage and proclaim This
beer is god. As far as we know,
other primates are not doing this.
The genetic source
We
can think of the human baby in it’s first lew years as living examples of some
of these physical changes taking place. At birth, the baby’s brain is onlya
quarter of it’s eventual weight and the larynx is much higher in the throat,
allowing babieslike chimpanzees, to breath and drink at the same time. In a
relative short period of time, the larynx
descends, the brain develop, the child assumes an upright posture and
starts walking and talking.
The
almost automatic set of development and the complexity of the young child’s
language have led some scholars to look for. Something more powerfull than
small physical adaptations of the species over time as the source of language.
Even children who are born deaf (and do not develop speech0 become fluent sign
language users, given appropriate circumstances. very early in life. This seems
to indicate that human offspring are born with a special capacity for language.
It is innate, no other creature seems to have it, and it isn’t tied to a
specific variety of language. Is it possible that this language capacity is
genetically hard-wired in the newborn human?
As
a solution to the puzzle of the origins of language, this innateness hypothesis
would seem to poit to something in human genetics, possibly a crucial mutation,
as the source. We are not sure when this proposed genetic change might have
taken place or how it might relate to the physical adaptatios described
earlier. However, as we consider this hypothesis, we find our speculations
about the origins of language moving away from fossil evidence or the physical
source of basic human sounds toward analogies with how computers (e.g. being
pre-programmed or hard-wired) and concepts taken from the study of genetics.
The investigations of the origins of language then turns into a search for
special “language gene” that only humans posssess.
If
we are indeed the only creatures with this special capacity for language, then
will it be completely impossible for any other creature to produce or
understand language? We’ll try to answer that question in Chapter 2.
Class : Introduction to linguistic
Lecturer : Dra. Nurprihatina Hasan, M.Hum
Semester
: First
Source : Lessson material photocopy
Comments
Post a Comment